Selected Abstracts: Russell W. Anderson


For reprints, please contact:
Russell W. Anderson
rwa@hnc.com


Table of Contents


Last Update: September 1998

R.W. Anderson, J.B. Badler, and E.L. Keller
Predicting distributions of neural connections in the saccadic system using a biologically plausible learning rule -- preliminary results. In: Evolutionary Programming VII: Proceedings of the Seventh Annual Conference on Evolutionary Programming (EP98), San Diego, CA, March 25-27, 1998. V.W. Porto, N. Saravanan, D.E. Waagen, and A.E. Eiben (eds.), Springer, Berlin, (1998). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 1447 pp. 503-513


R.W. Anderson, M.S. Ascher, and H.W. Sheppard
Direct HIV cytopathicity cannot account for CD4 decline in AIDS in the presence of homeostasis. A worst-case dynamical analysis
J. AIDS & Human Retrovirology,17:245-252 (1998).


R.W. Anderson, S. Das, and E.L.Keller. Estimation of Spatio-temporal Neural Activity Using Radial Basis Function Networks. J. Computational Neuroscience , 5:1-21 (1998).

R.W. Anderson, E.L. Keller, N.J. Gandhi, and S. Das Two-dimensional saccade-related population activity in superior colliculus in monkey. J. Neurophysiology 80:798-817 (1998).

R.W. Anderson.
How adaptive antibodies facilitate the evolution of natural antibodies.
Immunology and Cell Biology, 74(2):286-291 (1996).


M.S. Ascher, H.W. Sheppard, R.W. Anderson, J.F. Krowka, and H.J. Bremermann.
HIV results in the frame -- Paradox Remains.
Nature, 375:196 (1995).


R.W. Anderson.
The Baldwin effect.
In: Handbook of Evolutionary Computation Bock, T., D. Fogel, and Z. Michalewicz, eds. NY: Oxford University Press and IOP Publishing (1997)


R.W. Anderson
Random-walk learning: A neurobiological correlate to trial-and-error
Neural Networks and Pattern Recognition, OM Omidvar & J. Dayhoff, eds., Academic Press: Boston, Chapter 7, p. 221-244 (1988).


R.W. Anderson. "Learning and evolution:
A quantitative genetics approach
J. of Theoretical Biology, 175:89-101 (1995).


R.W. Anderson
On the maternal transmission of immunity: A "molecular attention" hypothesis
Biosystems, 34(1-3):87-105 (1995).


R.W. Anderson, A.U. Neumann and A.S. Perelson
A Cayley tree immune network model with antibody dynamics
Bull. Math. Biol., 55(6):1091-1131 (1993).


R.W. Anderson and V. Vemuri
Neural networks can be used for open-loop, dynamic control
Int'l. J. Neural Networks, 3(3):71-84 (1992).


H.J. Bremermann and R.W. Anderson
Mathematical models of HIV infection. I: Threshold conditions for transmission and host survival
J. of AIDS, 3(12):1129-1134 (1990).


F.U. Dowla, S.R. Taylor and R.W. Anderson
Seismic discrimination with artificial neural networks: Preliminary results with regional spectral data.
Bull. Seismo. Soc. Amer., 80(5):1346-1373 (1990).



R.W. Anderson, J.B. Badler, and E.L. Keller
Predicting distributions of neural connections in the saccadic system using a biologically plausible learning rule -- preliminary results. In: Evolutionary Programming VII: Proceedings of the Seventh Annual Conference on Evolutionary Programming (EP98), San Diego, CA, March 25-27, 1998. V.W. Porto, N. Saravanan, D.E. Waagen, and A.E. Eiben (eds.), Springer, Berlin, (1998). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 1447 pp. 503-513

Saccades are rapid movements that reposition the eye in space. Several neural structures involved in saccadic control have been characterized, providing a unique opportunity for systems-level investigations of the premotor neural circuitry. This study focuses on the role of the superior colliculus (SC) in the planning and control of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. Saccade-related neural activity in the SC is highly distributed, with saccade displacement commands coded in a topologically- organized motor map (Robinson 1972; Ottes et al. 1986). Downstream from the SC, this spatiotemporal code is transformed into the temporal code necessary to drive the oculomotor neurons. Researchers have postulated that this transformation is implemented in the projection weights between the SC and the brainstem saccadic burst generator. Here, an empirical neural network study is used to predict the topological variation of these projection weights. Estimates of the spatio-temporal neural activity in the SC were used as the open-loop inputs to the model. The projection weights from the SC to excitatory burst neurons (EBNs) in the brainstem were trained using a biologically plausible evolutionary learning rule (the chemotaxis algorithm), while well-known features of the downstream neural structures were fixed. The objective function was defined as the squared error between the model output and actual eye position trajectories for several magnitudes of horizontal saccades (integrated over time). Simulation results predict the excitatory connections from the SC to EBNs increase in strength or density with collicular location (from rostral to caudal).



R.W. Anderson, M.S. Ascher, and H.W. Sheppard
Direct HIV cytopathicity cannot account for CD4 decline in AIDS in the presence of homeostasis. A worst-case dynamical analysis J. AIDS & Human Retrovirology, 17:245-252 (1998)

The central paradox of HIV pathogenesis is that the viral burden (either free or cellular) seems far too low to deplete the CD4 population by direct killing. Until recently, there were few data which could be used to critically compare direct and indirect pathogenic theories. Clinical trials with potent new antiviral agents have measured important kinetic parameters of HIV infection, including viral and infected cell half-lives. This has led to the construction of explicit models of direct killing. Using a worst-case dynamical analysis, we show that such cytopathic models are untenable. Rates of infected cell removal are orders of magnitude too low to significantly suppress steady state CD4 counts in the face of lymphocyte replenishment, especially in early infection. Furthermore, the direct cytopathic models, as proposed, predict an extremely variable disease course across the broad range of observed viral burdens (five orders of magnitude), which is inconsistent with the relatively small differences in disease progression observed between patients. In contrast, immunological theories of pathogenesis, such as homeostatic dysregulation based on immune activation, do not suffer from these difficulties and are more consistent with the natural history of HIV infection.



R.W. Anderson, E.L. Keller, N.J. Gandhi, and S. Das
Two-dimensional saccade-related population activity in superior colliculus in monkey.
J. Neurophysiology 80:798-817 (1998).

The two-dimensional distribution of population activity in the superior colliculus (SC) during saccadic eye movements in the monkey was estimated using radial basis functions. In order to make these activity estimates, cells in the deeper layer of the SC were recorded over much of this structure. The dynamic movement field of each cell was determined at 2-ms intervals around the time of saccades for a wide variety of horizontal and oblique movements. Collicular neurons were divided into an overlapping dorsal burst neuron layer and a ventral buildup neuron layer. Cell location on the colliculus was estimated from the optimal target vector for a cell's visual response rather than from the optimal motor vector. The former technique that was found to be more reliable for locating some buildup neurons because it compared well with locations suggested by electrical stimulation. From the movement field data and from the estimates of each cell's anatomic location, the same algorithm was used to compute the two-dimensional population activity in the two layers of the SC during horizontal and oblique saccades. A subset of the sample of neurons, located near the horizontal meridian of the SC, was used to compute one-dimensional dynamic population activity estimates for horizontal saccades to allow direct comparison to previous studies. Statistical analyses on the one-dimensional data indicated that, while there is a shift in the center of gravity of the distributed activity in the buildup layer, there is little support for the theory of a systematic rostral spread of the activity that reaches the rostral limit of the colliculus at saccade end. The two-dimensional results extend the previous one-dimensional estimates of collicular activity. Discharge in the burst layer was found to be invariant in the size with saccade vector and symmetrically arranged about a center of gravity that did not move during saccades. The size of the active area in the buildup layer grew modestly with saccade amplitude while the distribution of activity was skewed toward the rostral end of the SC for saccade larger than 10. There was a clear shift in the center of gravity of the activity that was directed along the horizontal or an oblique meridian of the SC. However, the spread of activity during a saccade was as large or larger in the mediolateral direction as it was in the rostral direction. The results indicate changes in activity occur in an extended zone on the SC in all directions but caudal in the buildup layer during saccades and do not support the idea of a rostrally directed spread of activity as a dynamic control mechanism for saccades.



R.W. Anderson, S. Das, and E.L.Keller.
Estimation of Spatio-temporal Neural Activity Using Radial Basis Function Networks.
J. Computational Neuroscience , 5:1-21 (1998).

We report a method using radial basis function (RBF) networks to estimate the time evolution of population activity in topologically-organized neural structures from single-neuron recordings. This is an important problem in neuroscience research, as such estimates may provide insights into systems-level function of these structures. Since single-unit neural data tends to be unevenly sampled and highly variable under identical behavioral conditions, obtaining such estimates is a difficult task. Therefore, we have developed several computational geometry based algorithms for the initial treatment of the data before computing a surface estimation using RBF networks. To illustrate the use of these algorithms, our method is first applied to estimating the movement fields of neurons recorded in the deeper layers of the monkey superior colliculus during rapid (saccadic) eye movements. The method is then expanded to the problem of estimating simultaneous spatio-temporal activity occuring across the superior colliculus during a single movement (the inverse problem). In principle, this methodology could be applied to any neural structure with a regular, two-dimensional organization, provided a sufficient spatial distribution of sampled neurons is available.



R.W. Anderson.
How the adaptive antibodies facilitate the evolution of natural antibodies.
Immunology and Cell Biology, 74(2):286-291 (1996).

I show how Ig specificities, randomly generated in conventional B cells, can come to be expressed in the genetically-determinate B1 population. Thus the adaptive antibody population facilitates the evolution of the natural antibody repertoire, in accordance with the Baldwin effect in the evolution of instinct. I discuss the evolution of these two populations under both the "proximal usage" and "preferential expression" hypotheses of biased Ig gene segment usage. This process is independent of theories of B1 function.



M.S. Ascher, H.W. Sheppard, R.W. Anderson, J.F. Krowka, and H.J. Bremermann.
HIV results in the frame -- Paradox Remains.
Nature, 375:196 (1995).

SIR -- The articles by Ho et al.(1) and Wei et al. (2) have been hailed as providing crucial new information that clarifies the enigma of HIV-mediated pathogenesis (see, for example, refs 3,4). To the extent that they have estimated equilibrium rate constants and provided an explanation for rapid development of drug resistance, these studies (1,2) do provide new and important information. But the central paradox of AIDS pathogenesis remains.

The new studies on the dynamics of HIV infection demonstrate that the underlying rate of virus production is still 4-20 times lower than the rate of cell turnover. Because each infected cell produces many virions, and a high proportion of them are known to be defective, there is about 100-1,000 fold more cell death than can be accounted for by the observed rate of virus production (5). It is a murder scene with far more bodies than bullets.

Instead of addressing this discrepancy, the authors (1,2) suggest that "virus must be underestimated," or that many more infected cells must be hiding in deep lymphoid organs where they cannot be observed. Taken at face value, however, these data demonstrate that the CD4 lymphocyte population highly activated and that the fate of uninfected cells may be much more important in AIDS pathogenesis than that of infected cells. This view is consistent with observations that the rate of spontaneous apoptosis in the peripheral lymphocytes of HIV infected individuals is both sharply elevated and at least 10-100 fold higher than the frequency of productively infected cells (6).

Ho et al. suggest the analogy of a sink, with the tap and drain both equally wide open, which eventually empties because the "regenerative capacity of the immune system (the tap) is not infinite" and can't quite keep up. However, the differential between the tap and drain is extremely small (20-200 x 10^6 cells/day) compared with the overall flow rate (2 x 10^9 cells/day), and must remain relatively fixed for an average of 10 years, as CD4+ cell loss is roughly linear throughout most of the natural history of HIV infection (7). Further, this model would predict no CD4 loss until virus production exceeded a critical threshold, and then an accelerating cell loss as the virus burden increases.

A more plausible explanation for these data is that a mechanism which finely regulates CD4 replacement makes a slight error, resulting in the failure to completely replace the cells (infected and uninfected alike) which are lost through programmed cell death, the natural consequence of immune activation. We have argued that this is exactly what would be expected if the immune system were exposed to a persistent co-stimulatory T-cell signal caused by the interaction of gp120 with CD4 (ref. 8). This extra signal causes the control mechanism to sense an elevated state of immune responsiveness and downregulate the recovery of "memory" cells to prevent growth of the immune system (9). The result would be an inexorable but steady decay based on this difference in probability of survival.

Maddox (4) wonders "why, after more than a decade of research, has it only now emerged that the response of the immune system to infection by HIV is hyperactivity rather than the opposite?". The answer to this question is that those who would see AIDS as a more-or-less conventional viral infection have consistently refused to recognize the paradoxes that are clearly evident in the experimental data. The problem continues.

References
1. Ho, D.D., A.U. Neumann, A.S. Perelson, W. Chen, J.M. Leonard, and M. Markowitz, Rapid turnover of plasma virions and CD4 lymphocytes in HIV-1 infection. Nature, 373: 123-126 (1995).
2. Wei, X., S.K. Goush, M.E. Taylor, et al. Viral dynamics in human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection. Nature, 373:117--122 (1995).
3. Wain-Hobson, S. (1995). Virological mayhem. Nature, 373:102 (1995).
4. Maddox, J. Nature 373,189 (1995)
5. Sheppard, H.W., M.S. Ascher, and J.F. Krowka Viral burden and HIV disease. Nature, 364:291-92.Nature 364,291 (1993).
6. Gougeon, M.L. and Montagnier, L. Apoptosis in AIDS. Science, 260:1269-1270 (1993). (published erratum appears in Science, 260:1709 (1993))
7. Sheppard, H.W. et al. J. AIDS 7,1159-1166 (1993).
8. Ascher, M.S. and Sheppard, H.W. AIDS as immune system activation II: the panergic imnesia hypothesis J. AIDS, 3:177-191 (1990).
9. Sheppard, H.W. and M.S. Ascher, The natural history and pathogenesis of HIV infection. Annu. Rev. Microbiol., 46:533-564 (1992).


R.W. Anderson.
The Baldwin effect.
In: Handbook of Evolutionary Computation
Bock, T., D. Fogel, and Z. Michalewicz, eds. NY: Oxford University Press and IOP Publishing (1997)

Abstract

The Baldwin effect is a passive evolutionary process, whereby individual learning facilitates genetic evolution. Baldwinian evolution is distinguished from the more active (and non-biological) Lamarckian inheritance of acquired characters. This chapter explains the principles underlying the Baldwin effect and discusses its manifestations in evolutionary algorithms. A first-order analysis using quantitative genetics is used to illustrate some common misconceptions. When appropriately implemented, hybrid algorithms can efficiently exploit the Baldwin effect in evolutionary optimization.

1. Interactions Between Learning and Evolution
In the course of an evolutionary optimization, solutions are often generated with low phenotypic fitness even though the corresponding genotype may be close to an optima. Without additional information about the local fitness landscape, such genetic ``near misses" would be overlooked under strong selection. Presumably, one could rank near misses by performing a local search and scoring them according to distance from the nearest optima. Such evaluations are essentially the goal of hybrid algorithms (Chapter B1.5; Balakrishnan and Honavar 1995), which combine global search using evolutionary algorithms and local search using individual learning algorithms. Hybrid algorithms can exploit learning either actively (via Lamarckian inheritance) or passively (via the Baldwin effect).

Under Lamarckian algorithms, performance gains from individual learning are mapped back into the genotype used for the production of the next generation. This is analogous to Lamarckian inheritance in evolutionary theory --- whereby characters acquired during a parent's lifetime are passed on to their offspring. Lamarckian inheritance is rejected as a biological mechanism under the Modern Synthesis, since it is difficult to envision a process by which acquired information can be transferred into the gametes. Nevertheless, the practical utility of Lamarckian algorithms has been demonstrated in some evolutionary optimization applications (Ackley and Littman 1994; Paechter et al. 1995). Of course, these algorithms are limited to problems where a reverse mapping from the learned phenotype to genotype is possible.

However even under purely Darwinian selection, individual learning influences evolutionary processes, but the underlying mechanisms are subtle. The ``Baldwin effect" is one such mechanism, whereby learning facilitates the assimilation of new genetic innovations (Baldwin 1896; Morgan 1896; Osborn 1896; Waddington 1942; Hinton and Nowlan 1987; Maynard Smith 1987; Anderson 1995a; Turney et al. 1996). Learning allows an individual to complete and exploit partial genetic programs and thereby survive. In other words, learning guides evolution by assigning ``partial credit" for genetic near misses. Individuals with useful genetic variations are thus maintained by learning, and the corresponding genes increase in frequency in the subsequent generation. As genetic components necessary for a complex structure accumulate in the gene pool, functions that previously required supplemental learning are replaced by genetically-determinant systems.

Empirical studies can quantify the benefits of incorporating individual learning into evolutionary algorithms (Belew 1989; French and Messinger 1994; Nolfi et al. 1994; Whitley et al. 1994; Cecconi et al. 1995). However, a theoretical treatment of the effects of learning on evolution can strengthen our intuition for when and how to implement such approaches. This chapter presents an overview of the principles underlying the Baldwin effect, beginning with a brief history of the elucidation and development in evolutionary biology. Computational models of the Baldwin effect are reviewed and critiqued. The Baldwin effect is then analyzed using standard quantitative genetics. Given reasonable assumptions of the effects of learning on fitness and its associated costs, this theoretical approach builds and strengthens conventional intuition about the effects of individual learning on evolution. Finally, issues concerning problem formulation, learning algorithms, and algorithmic design are discussed.





R.W. Anderson
Random-walk learning: A neurobiological correlate to trial-and-error
Progress in Neural Networks, O.M. Omidvar and J. Dayhoff, Eds., Academic Press: Boston, Chapter 7,p.221-244 (1998).

Neural network models offer a theoretical testbed for the study of learning at the cellular level. The only experimentally verified learning rule, Hebb's rule, is extremely limited in its ability to train networks to perform complex tasks. An identified cellular mechanism responsible for Hebbian-type long-term potentiation, the NMDA receptor, is highly versatile. Its function and efficacy are modulated by a wide variety of compounds and conditions and are likely to be directed by non-local phenomena. Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that NMDA receptors are not essential for some types of learning. We have shown that another neural network learning rule, the chemotaxis algorithm, is theoretically much more powerful than Hebb's rule and is consistent with experimental data. A biased random-walk in synaptic weight space is a learning rule immanent in nervous activity and may account for some types of learning -- notably the acquisition of skilled movement.



R.W. Anderson.
Learning and evolution: A quantitative genetics approach
J. of Theoretical Biology, 175:89-101 (1995).

Recent models of the interactions between learning and evolution show that learning increases the rate at which populations find optima in fixed environments. However, learning ability is only advantageous in variable environments. In this study, quantitative genetics models are used to investigate the effects of individual learning on evolution. Two models of populations of learning individuals are constructed and analyzed. In the first model, the effect of learning is represented as an increase in the variance of selection. Dynamical equations and equilibrium conditions are derived for a population of learning individuals under fixed and variable environmental selection. In the second model, the amount of individual learning effort is regulated by a second gene specifying the duration of a critical learning period. The second model includes a model of the learning process to determine the individual fitness costs and benefits accrued during the learning period. Individuals are then selected for the optimal learning investment. The similarities of the results from these two models suggest that the net effects of learning on evolution are relatively independent of the mechanisms underlying the learning process.


R.W. Anderson
On the maternal transmission of immunity: A "molecular attention" hypothesis
Biosystems, 34(1-3):87-105 (1995).

Maternally-derived antibodies can provide passive protection to their offspring. More subtle phenomena associated with maternal antibodies concern their influence in shaping the immune repertoire and priming the neonatal immune response. These phenomena suggest that maternal antibodies play a role in the education of the neonatal immune system. The educational effects are thought to be mediated by idiotypic interactions among antibodies and B cells in the context of an idiotypic network. This paper proposes that maternal antibodies trigger localized idiotypic network activity that serves to amplify and translate information concerning the molecular shapes of potential antigens. The triggering molecular signals are contained in the binding regions of the antibody molecules. These antibodies form complexes and are taken up by antigen presenting cells or retained by follicular dendritic cells and thereby incorporated into more traditional cellular immune memory mechanisms. This mechanism for maternal transmission of immunity is termed the molecular attention hypothesis and is contrasted to the dynamic memory hypothesis. Experiments are proposed that may help indicate which models are more appropriate and will further our understanding of these intriguing natural phenomena. Finally, analogies are drawn to attention in neural systems.



R.W. Anderson, A.U. Neumann and A.S. Perelson
A Cayley tree immune network model with antibody dynamics
Bull. Math. Biol., 55(6):1091-1131 (1993).

A Cayley tree model of idiotypic networks that includes both B cell and antibody dynamics is formulated and analysed. As in models with B cells only, localized states exist in the network with limited numbers of activated clones surrounded by virgin or near-virgin clones. The existence and stability of these localized network states are explored as a function of model parameters. As in previous models that have included antibody, the stability of immune and tolerant localized states are shown to depend on the ratio of antibody to B cell lifetimes as well as the rate of antibody complex removal. As model parameters are varied, localized steady-states can break down via two routes: dynamically, into chaotic attractors, or structurally into percolation attractors. For a given set of parameters percolation and chaotic attractors can coexist with localized attractors, and thus there do not exist clear cut boundaries in parameter space that separate regions of localized attractors from regions of percolation and chaotic attractors. Stable limit cycles, which are frequent in the two-clone antibody B cell (AB) model, are only observed in highly connected networks. Also found in highly connected networks are localized chaotic attractors. As in experiments by Lundkvist et al. (1989. Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 5074-5078), injection of Ab1 antibodies into a system operating in the chaotic regime can cause a cessation of fluctuations of Ab1 and Ab2 antibodies, a phenomenon already observed in the two-clone AB model. Interestingly, chaotic fluctuations continue at higher levels of the tree, a phenomenon observed by Lundkvist et al. but not accounted for previously.



R.W. Anderson and V. Vemuri
Neural networks can be used for open-loop, dynamic control
Int'l. J. Neural Networks, 3(3):71-84 (1992).

The use of artificial neural nets to generate control signals for dynamical systems is investigated. Thus far, attempts to apply neural nets to temporal signal generation have met with limited success. Training an unconstrained, 'recurrent' network of processing nodes to generate even the most elementary temporal patterns appears to be prohibitively slow. However, theoretical analyses of static neural networks demonstrate their power as convenient interpolation devices. This property is exploited by training the simpler, 'feed-forward' neural networks to generate the key parameters of the optimal control signal, namely, the switching times. This methodology is applied to problems in optimal motor control in two stages. First, a network is trained to generate the appropriate optimal switching times. Second, the networks are trained directly on the resultant dynamical state trajectory. The latter experiment relies exclusively on the chemotaxis algorithm, since back-propagation would require calculation of the partial derivatives back through an unknown plant.



Hans J. Bremermann and R.W. Anderson
Mathematical models of HIV infection. I: Threshold conditions for transmission and host survival
J. of AIDS, 3(12):1129-1134 (1990).

This is the second in a series of papers modeling HIV infections at four interacting levels: transmission, interaction with the immune system, gene regulation, and selection of mutants. In the previous paper (Bremermann and Anderson [1989]) models have been motivated and described verbally and a conjecture about the HIV cytopathic effect, based upon the models (and a review of the literature) has been stated. In the following we write mathematical equations about threshold conditions which connect infectivity, length of host survival, and frequency of acts conducive to transmission. The formula is derived not only for homogeneous populations, but also for populations of an arbitrary number of subgroups with different frequencies of risk behavior, different infectivities and latency periods, and different interaction frequencies with other groups.




F.U. Dowla, S.R. Taylor and R.W. Anderson
Seismic discrimination with artificial neural networks: Preliminary results with regional spectral data
Bull. Seismo. Soc. Amer., 80(5):1346-1373 (1990).

An application of artificial neural networks (ANN) for discrimination between natural earthquakes and underground nuclear explosions has been studied using distance corrected spectral data of regional seismic phases. Pn, Pg, and Lg spectra have been analyzed from 83 western U.S. earthquakes and 87 Nevada Test Site explosions recorded at the four broadband seismic stations operated by Lawrence Livermore National Laboratory. Distance corrections are applied to the raw spectral data using existing frequency-dependent Q models for the Basin and Range. The spectra are sampled logarithmically at 41 points between 0.1 and 10 Hz for each phase and checked for adequate signal-to-noise ratios (S/N >2). The ANN was implemented on a SUN 4/110 workstation using a backpropagation-feedforward architecture. We find that, using even simple ANN architectures (82 input units, 1 hidden unit, and 2 output units), powerful discrimination systems can be designed. In order to regionalize the data characteristics, a separate neural network was assigned to each station. For this data set, the rate of correct recognition for untrained data is over 93 per cent for both earthquakes and explosions at any single station. Using a majority voting scheme with a network of four stations, the rate of recognition is over 97 per cent. Although the performance of the ANN is similar to that of the Fisher linear discriminant, the ANN exhibits a number of computational advantages over the conventional method. Finally, examination of the network weights suggests that, in addition to spectral shape, a criterion that the ANN utilized to discriminate between the two populations was the Lg/Pg spectral amplitude ratios.


Top of Page
Russell Anderson's Home Page
Go to the Athens GeoCities
Author
Russell Anderson (rwa@milo.berkeley.edu)

Created: anderson - February 1996